(logo) Natural Genesis (logo text)
A Sourcebook for the Worldwide Discovery of a Creative Organic Universe
Table of Contents
Introduction
Genesis Vision
Learning Planet
Organic Universe
Earth Life Emerge
Genesis Future
Glossary
Recent Additions
Search
Submit

V. Life's Corporeal Evolution Develops, Encodes and Organizes Itself: An EarthWinian Genesis Synthesis

7. Multiple Ancestries of Homo Sapiens

Tattersall, Ian. Once We Were Not Alone. Scientific American. January, 2000. Recent views on the origins of Homo Sapiens. Until some 150,000 years ago Africa was populated by other hominids. The winner gained advantage by symbolic thought made possible through language.

Tattersall, Ian. The Monkey in the Mirror. New York: Harcourt, 2001. The curator of anthropology at the American Museum of Natural History sees the acquisition of language and symbolic art as the prime cognitive abilities that distinguish the human presence. But ascribing to the old materialist mindset, Tattersall takes a very dour view saying that nature never intended human beings, people are just here as “accidental tourists.”

Tibayrenc, Michel and Francisco Ayala, eds. On Human Nature: Biology, Psychology, Ethics, Politics, and Religion. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, 2016. The 46 chapter compendium edited by the French research physician and the UC Irvine philosophical biologist spans three parts: Biological Basis of Human Diversity, Psychology, Behavior and Society, and Ethics, Politics and Religious Considerations. An Abstract for the lead chapter The Advent of Biological Evolution and Humankind by Ayala and Spanish philosopher Camilo Cela Conde is below. Some other titles are The Human Family: Evolutionary Origins and Adaptive Significance, Human Life History Evolution, Hominins: Context, Origins, and Taxic Diversity, Gene-Culture Models for the Evolution of Altruistic Teaching, Human Sociology and Group Selection Theory, and Universal Humanity, Religious Particularity, and Scientific Reductionism. A broad range is covered but still sans any sense of a greater phenomenal genesis from which they spring form and exemplify. In this absence, the current major evolutionary emergent transition to a prodigious personsphere learning on her/his own is further removed.

The human lineage appeared around 7 Ma as the sister group of chimpanzees, our closest relatives. Thus, humans and chimpanzees are very similar, genetically speaking, though they differ in many conspicuous phenotypic, functional, and adaptive traits. What was the cause of the appearance of humanity? Is it related to hazardous episodes? Or, is it the result of a necessity due to the fact that our nature endows a somehow adaptive superior capacity that justifies the human prevalence among all primates? In this chapter we examine the components of chance and necessity in biological evolution. We will present the distinctive features of the human lineage and discuss to what extent the synapomorphic trait shared by the whole lineage of humans—bipedalism—can be considered a product of adaptive selection or random events. Finally, we introduce the current possibilities of analysis to characterize the origin of the derived features of Homo sapiens. (Abstract: The Advent of Biological Evolution and Humankind: Chance and Necessity? By C. J. Cela-Conde and F. J. Ayala)

Tomasello, Michael. A Natural History of Human Thinking. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 2014. As the publisher notes next, after two decades of field and laboratory research, the co-director of the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, with many colleagues, presents a fully developed theory of how peoples came to our cognitive abilities to do this. The passage from ape primates to hominids to homo sapiens can be best tracked and understood by degrees of cooperation and collaboration. With the chimpanzees, a phase of “individual intentionality” came to replace competition alone with informed communication. As groups evolved to the lineages of early man, a “joint intentionality” took precedent by way of “second-person thinking,” a communal discourse whence members becomes aware of each other’s mind and thought.

In spiral turn, with human beings a stage of “shared, collective intentionality” arises. Beyond genetic nuance, an increasing communal coherence and reciprocity distinguishes and propels our prolific, expansive, technological societies. As the fourth quote cites, this species trajectory is quite similar to the pace and sequence by which each child advances. So a long episode of evolving from me vs. me to We and me for mutual benefit, is now well defined and qunatified. With a major transition to a planetary progeny is lately underway, how might a worldwise “shared, collective cognition” be able reach its own salutary knowledge and intention in time to save us?


In this much-anticipated book, Michael Tomasello weaves his twenty years of comparative studies of humans and great apes into a compelling argument that cooperative social interaction is the key to our cognitive uniqueness. Once our ancestors learned to put their heads together with others to pursue shared goals, humankind was on an evolutionary path all its own. (He) argues that our prehuman ancestors, like today's great apes, were social beings who could solve problems by thinking. But they were almost entirely competitive, aiming only at their individual goals. As ecological changes forced them into more cooperative living arrangements, early humans had to coordinate their actions and communicate their thoughts with collaborative partners.

Tomasello's "shared intentionality hypothesis" captures how these more socially complex forms of life led to more conceptually complex forms of thinking. In order to survive, humans had to learn to see the world from multiple social perspectives, to draw socially recursive inferences, and to monitor their own thinking via the normative standards of the group. Even language and culture arose from the preexisting need to work together. What differentiates us most from other great apes, Tomasello proposes, are the new forms of thinking engendered by our new forms of collaborative and communicative interaction. (Publisher)

In their magisterial survey of life on planet earth, (John) Maynard-Smith and (Eors) Szathmary (1995) identified eight major transitions in the evolution of complexity of living things, for example, the emergence of chromosomes, the emergence of multicellular organisms, and the emergence of sexual reproduction. Astoundingly, in each case the transition was characterized by the same two fundamental processes. First, in each case there emerged some new form of cooperation with interdependence: “Entities that were capable of independent replication before the transition can replicate only as part of a larger whole after it” (6). Second, in each case this new form of cooperation was made possible by a concomitant new form of communication: “change in the method of information transmission” (6). (32)

There can be no doubt that the last common ancestor to humans and other primates engaged in individual thinking in pursuit of individual goals, mostly in order to compete with groupmates for valued resources. Along the way, they attended to situations relevant to those goals. Early human individuals – in response to a changing feeding ecology – then began to join together with other individuals dyadically in pursuit of joint goals, and they jointly amended to situations relevant to that joint goal. Each participant in the collaboration had her own individual role and her own individual perspective on the situation as part of the interactive unit. This dual-level structure – simultaneous jointness and individuality – is the defining structure of what we are calling “joint intentionality,” and it is foundational for all subsequent manifestations of human shared intentionality. (78)

First, although ontogeny does not have to recapitulate phylogeny, in the current case the relation between joint intentionality and collective intentionality is partly logical – one must have some skills for coordinating with other individuals before coordinating with the group – and so the ontogenetic ordering is basically the same as our hypothesized phylogenetic ordering. (144)

Tomasello, Michael. The Origins of Morality. Scientific American. September, 2018. The former director of the MPI Evolutionary Anthropology and prolific author has now moved back to alma mater Duke University. In a topical issue on The Science of Being Human, he summarizes his 2016 work A Natural History of Morality which cites reciprocal interdependence as the social quality which most facilitated our Homo sapiens advent. By similar concepts to Kevin Laland in this issue (above), a self-reinforcing collaborative intelligence and cooperation serves a relative group cohesion and knowledge. In a full page graphic, an evolutionary course proceeds from intentionality by Individual primates (me) to Joint hominids (We) onto a beneficial Shared cultural phase (US). A salutary lesson may then accrue, as the quote notes. We and me are seen as natural complements, and We should take precedence over me. However, one wonders, might these anthropological insights transfer and be availed by public politics where they are locked in brutal conflict. See also Profile of Michael Tomasello and How Children Come to Understand False Beliefs: A Shared Intentionality Account in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (115/34, 2018).

The essence of this account is thus a kind of “we is greater than me” Psychological orientation, which gives moral notions their special of legitimacy in personal decision making. (75) But if we are to solve our largest challenges as a species, which threaten all human societies alike, we had best be prepared to think of all of humanity as a “we.” (75)

Tomasello, Michael and Esther Herrmann. Ape and Human Cognition: What’s the Difference? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 19/1, 2010. Max Planck Institute evolutionary anthropologists first aver our deep mental continuity with primate ancestors. What distinguishes us however is a social culture so intensely interlinked as to take on a communal, organism-like guise capable of its own cerebral intention.

Humans share the vast majority of their cognitive skills with other great apes. In addition, however, humans have also evolved a unique sense of cognitive skills and motivations – collectively referred to as shared intentionality – for living collaboratively, learning socially, and exchanging information in cultural groups. (3) Virtually all of humans’ highest cognitive achievements are not the work of individuals acting alone but rather of individuals collaborating in groups. (5)

Tomasello, Michael and Hannes Rakoczy. What Makes Human Cognition Unique? From Individual to Shared to Collective Intentionality. Mind & Language. 18/2, 2003. From the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, a hypothesis that distinctive human faculties are not due just to brain size but arise from the complex cultural milieu in which homo sapiens became immersed. This theory is based on studies of how children proceed in their first four years from an infant self-emphasis to recognizing others as mental agents and onto a collective social intentionality. The tacit assumption is that a child repeats the same developmental stages as the human species passed through.

Tomasello, Michael, et al. Two Key Steps in the Evolution of Human Cooperation: The Interdependence Hypothesis. Current Anthropology. 53/6, 2012. In an article following Gowlett, et al, above, in this issue, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology researchers offer explanations for our inveterate proclivity to join into viable societies. Beyond unselfish altruistic concern, a pervasive reciprocity among members, read “mutual aid, ubuntu, creative union” and so on, is a real, major factor in their success. That is to say, a natural complementarity of Me and We, rather than withering conflict, advised by a common “intentionality,” is good for both self and group.


Modern theories of the evolution of human cooperation focus mainly on altruism. In contrast, we propose that humans’ species-unique forms of cooperation—as well as their species-unique forms of cognition, communication, and social life—all derive from mutualistic collaboration (with social selection against cheaters). In a first step, humans became obligate collaborative foragers such that individuals were interdependent with one another and so had a direct interest in the well-being of their partners. In this context, they evolved new skills and motivations for collaboration not possessed by other great apes (joint intentionality), and they helped their potential partners (and avoided cheaters). In a second step, these new collaborative skills and motivations were scaled up to group life in general, as modern humans faced competition from other groups. As part of this new group-mindedness, they created cultural conventions, norms, and institutions (all characterized by collective intentionality), with knowledge of a specific set of these marking individuals as members of a particular cultural group. Human cognition and sociality thus became ever more collaborative and altruistic as human individuals became ever more interdependent. (Abstract)

In support of our hypotheses, we focus on two sources of evidence not common in anthropology. First, we invoke experimental studies, many from our own laboratory, that compare the cognitive and motivational skills of humans, mostly young children, and their nearest great ape relatives (as representative, in a very general way, of the last common ancestor). We show that even young children are adapted for collaborative activities in a way that other great apes are not. Second, we also in some cases invoke human ontogenetic sequences as suggestive of potential phylogenetic sequences, for example, that young children in fact (and possibly of logical necessity) learn to collaborate with other individuals in concrete situations before they construct more abstract group-level phenomena such as social norms and institutions. (674)

Tuttle, Russell. Apes and Human Evolution. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 2014. Drawing upon a lifetime of “field and experimental laboratory studies pertaining to the evolution of human and nonhuman primate morphology, locomotion, and other behavior” (his UC website), the University of Chicago anthropologist provides a current, inclusive synopsis of how monkeys become men.

In this masterwork, Russell H. Tuttle synthesizes a vast research literature in primate evolution and behavior to explain how apes and humans evolved in relation to one another, and why humans became a bipedal, tool-making, culture-inventing species distinct from other hominoids. Along the way, he refutes the influential theory that men are essentially killer apes--sophisticated but instinctively aggressive and destructive beings. Situating humans in a broad context, Tuttle musters convincing evidence from morphology and recent fossil discoveries to reveal what early primates ate, where they slept, how they learned to walk upright, how brain and hand anatomy evolved simultaneously, and what else happened evolutionarily to cause humans to diverge from their closest relatives. Despite our genomic similarities with bonobos, chimpanzees, and gorillas, humans are unique among primates in occupying a symbolic niche of values and beliefs based on symbolically mediated cognitive processes. (Publisher)

Tylen, Kristian, et al. The Evolution of Early Symbolic Behavior in Homo Sapiens.. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 117/4578, 2020. We cite this entry by Aarhus University, University of Johannesburg, and University of Western Australia archaeologists to convey, along with bodily features, how life’s course to our late cognitive-social phase brought out the first signs of an external depiction and record of their vital environments. Another instance is the finding of earlier cave paintings in Indonesia (Google) of an animal hunt.

Early symbolic behavior of Homo sapiens is challenging to address yet fundamental to the success of our species. We used ancient engravings from the South African Blombos Cave and Diepkloof Rock Shelter in a number of controlled cognitive experiments to qualify discussions about the evolution of early symbolic traditions. We found that the engravings evolved over a period of 30,000 y to become more effective “tools for the mind,” that is, more salient to the human eye, expressive of human intent and identity, and easier to reproduce from memory. Our experiments suggest that the engravings served as decorations and expressions of socially transmitted cultural traditions. (Significance)

Ullrich, Herbert, ed. Hominid Evolution. Berlin: Archaea, 1999. Forty papers from a 1997 meeting of the Human Evolution International Interdisciplinary Project, a collective effort to reconstruct humankind came to be. Emphasis is on descriptive aspects such as adaptation strategies, hunting, subsistence and incipient culture from primates to Cro Magnon.

Veeramah, Krishna and Michael Hammer. The Impact of Whole-Genome Sequencing on the Reconstruction of Human Population History. Nature Reviews Genetics. 15/3, 2014. University of Arizona biologists describe how these novel techniques can help revive the cast of characters who have came and went on the circuitous course to we “anatomical moderns.” By way of multilocus data, single-nucleotide polymorphisms, non-recombining Y chromosomes, and so on, extinct hominins can be identified, along with their migratory paths from Africa. One wonders, whom then is this late planetary personage now altogether able to do this? What kind of dynamic universe is this that billions of years on, via an earthly biosphere and human noosphere, is trying to achieve its own sentient description and witness?

Examining patterns of molecular genetic variation in both modern-day and ancient humans has proved to be a powerful approach to learn about our origins. Rapid advances in DNA sequencing technology have allowed us to characterize increasing amounts of genomic information. Although this clearly provides unprecedented power for inference, it also introduces more complexity into the way we use and interpret such data. Here, we review ongoing debates that have been influenced by improvements in our ability to sequence DNA and discuss some of the analytical challenges that need to be overcome in order to fully exploit the rich historical information that is contained in the entirety of the human genome. (Abstract)

[Prev Pages]   Previous   | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12  Next