![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
|
![]() |
![]() |
||||||||||
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
|
IV. Ecosmomics: An Independent, UniVersal, Source Code-Script of Generative Complex Network Systems2. The Innate Affinity of Genomes, Protenomes and Language Eetemadi, Ameen and Ilias Tagkopoulos. Genetic Neural Networks: An Artificial Neural Network Architecture for Capturing Gene Expression Relationships. Bioinformatics. 35/13, 2019. We cite this entry by UC Davis computer scientists to show how readily these popular analytic methods seem to find similar application everywhere, even in this case so as to parse life’s heredity. Could commonality infer that brains and genomes and all else are deeply cerebral, information bearing, relative aware in kind? Results: We present the Genetic Neural Network (GNN), an artificial neural network for predicting genome-wide gene expression given gene knockouts and master regulator perturbations. In its core, the GNN maps existing gene regulatory information in its architecture and it uses cell nodes that have been specifically designed to capture the dependencies and non-linear dynamics that exist in gene networks. Our results argue that GNNs can become the architecture of choice when building predictors of gene expression from the growing corpus of genome-wide transcriptomics data. Elnaggar, A., et al. ProtTrans: Towards Cracking the Language of Life’s Code Through Self-Supervised Deep Learning and High Performance Computing. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence. 14/8, 2021. Twelve computer scientists mainly at the Technical University of Munich explore these 2020 frontiers of deep new methods and insights into the deep, natural grammars of the language of life9 from the text). A long Abstract cites many computer code methods with a facile ability facility to read, write and take up natural, ecosmomic code-scripts. See also CodeTrans: Towards Cracking the Language of Silicon's Code Through Self-Supervised Deep Learning and High Performance Computing by the same group at arXiv:2104.02443,. Faltynek, Dan, et al. Bases are not Letters: On the Analogy between the Genetic Code and Natural Language by Sequence Analysis. Biosemiotics. Online April, 2019. Palacky University, Olomouc, Czech Republic system scholars DF, Vladimir Matlach, and Ludmila Lackova (search) continue their project to parse an endemic, natural affinity between the prime informative occasions of biochemical nucleotide genomes and human linguistic complexities. The article deals with the notion of the genetic code and its metaphorical understanding as a “language”. In the traditional view of the language metaphor of the genetic code, combinations of nucleotides are signs of amino acids. Similarly, words combined from letters (speech sounds) represent certain meanings. The language metaphor of the genetic code assumes that the nucleotides stay in the analogy to letters, triples to words and genes to sentences. We propose an application of mathematical linguistic methods on the notion of the genetic code. We provide quantitative analysis (n-gram structure, Zipf’s law) of mRNA strings and natural language texts, along with a representative analysis of DNA, RNA and proteins. Our analysis of mRNA confirms an assumption that the design of the genetic code cannot analogize DNA bases and letters. The notion of the letter is much more appropriate if analogized with triplets or amino acids (Abstract excerpt) Ferrer-I-Cancho, Ramon and Nuria Forns. The Self-Organization of Genomes. Complexity. Online First, March, 2010. As the quote cites, Barcelona biologists contribute to the recent robust affirmation that genetic and linguistic codes are one and the same in their expression of the universal complex system dynamics, which then, one may add, could take on the likeness of an independent, mathematical cosmic genotype. Menzerath-Altmann law is a general law of human language stating, for instance, that the longer a word, the shorter its syllables. With the metaphor that genomes are words and chromosomes are syllables, we examine if genomes also obey the law. We find that longer genomes tend to be made of smaller chromosomes in organisms from three different kingdoms: fungi, plants, and animals. Our findings suggest that genomes self-organize under principles similar to those of human language. (Abstract) Ferrer-i-Cancho, Ramon, et al. The Challenges of Statistical Patterns of Language: The Case of Menzerath’s Law in Genomes. Complexity. Online December, 2012. With coauthors Nuria Forns, Antoni Hernandez-Fernandez, Gemma Bel-enguix and Jaume Baixeries, Barcelona systems scientists advise that along with (George Kingsley) Zipf’s law, the theorem of German linguist Paul Menzerath about word or note frequencies in a text or score can hold equally well for biomolecular nucleotide genomes. By these lights, another entry is gained to appreciate a deep, parallel affinity between the genetic code and literate languages. The importance of statistical patterns of language has been debated over decades. Although Zipf's law is perhaps the most popular case, recently, Menzerath's law has begun to be involved. Menzerath's law manifests in language, music and genomes as a tendency of the mean size of the parts to decrease as the number of parts increases in many situations. This statistical regularity emerges also in the context of genomes, for instance, as a tendency of species with more chromosomes to have a smaller mean chromosome size. It has been argued that the instantiation of this law in genomes is not indicative of any parallel between language and genomes because (a) the law is inevitable and (b) noncoding DNA dominates genomes. Here mathematical, statistical, and conceptual challenges of these criticisms are discussed. Two major conclusions are drawn: the law is not inevitable and languages also have a correlate of noncoding DNA. However, the wide range of manifestations of the law in and outside genomes suggests that the striking similarities between noncoding DNA and certain linguistics units could be anecdotal for understanding the recurrence of that statistical law. (Abstract) Ferruz, Noelia, et al. ProtGPT2 is a Deep Unsupervised Language Model for Protein Design. Nature Communications. 13/4348, 2022. University of Bayreuth, German system biochemists describe current progress toward a deep unity of life’s two prime genetic and linguistic code domains. By virtue of AI/ML facilities, into the 2020s an infinite affinity, as long sensed, is being revealed. This worldwise phase of palliative and enhanced metabolomics then brings much promise for health and welfare. Protein design projects aim to build novel biomolecules customized for specific purposes so to potentially solve many environmental and biomedical problems. Recent progress in Transformer-based architectures have been enabled by language models which can generate text with human-like capabilities. Here, we describe ProtGPT2, a linguistic form that can build de novo protein sequences following the principles of natural ones. AlphaFold prediction of ProtGPT2-sequences yield structures with embodiments and topologies not captured in current structure databases.
Flam-Sherperd, Daniel, et al.
Atom-by-atom protein generation and beyond with language models.
arXiv:2308.09482.
Protein language models learn powerful representations directly from sequences of amino acids. In contrast, chemical language models learn atom-level results of smaller molecules that include every atom, bond, and ring. In this work, we show that chemical language models can learn atom-level proteins which can generate the standard genetic code and far beyond it. The results demonstrate the potential for biomolecular design at the atom level using language models. (Exerpt) Gimona, Mario. Protein Linguistics and the Modular Code of the Cytoskeleton. Barbieri, Marcello, ed. The Codes of Life. Berlin: Springer, 2008. The University of Salzburg geneticist contributes to the long project to interpret, join and unify the molecular and literal versions, in support of the growing conclusion that “Nature is Structured in a Language-like Fashion.” See also an earlier paper “Protein Linguistics – A Grammar for Modular Protein Assembly?” in Nature Reviews: Molecular Cell Biology (7/1, 2006). Hackenberg, Michael, et al. Clustering of DNA Words and Biological Function: A Proof of Principle. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 297/127, 2012. University of Granada and University of Malaga, Spain system biologists including Pedro Carpena contribute to historic 2010s verifications that the molecular nucleotide version and human cultural literature are one and the same, that they are formed and suffused by the same informative nonlinear complex network systems. View articles of this kind, for example, in the journal Complexity over recent years. Relevant words in literary texts (key words) are known to be clustered, while common words are randomly distributed. Given the clustered distribution of many functional genome elements, we hypothesize that the biological text per excellence, the DNA sequence, might behave in the same way: k-length words (k-mers) with a clear function may be spatially clustered along the one-dimensional chromosome sequence, while less-important, non-functional words may be randomly distributed. To explore this linguistic analogy, we calculate a clustering coefficient for each k-mer (k=2–9 bp) in human and mouse chromosome sequences, then checking if clustered words are enriched in the functional part of the genome. The clustering of DNA words thus appears as a novel principle to detect functionality in genome sequences. As evolutionary conservation is not a prerequisite, the proof of principle described here may open new ways to detect species-specific functional DNA sequences and the improvement of gene and promoter predictions, thus contributing to the quest for function in the genome. (Abstract excerpt) Holzer, Jacqueline. Genomes & Language. http://www.liu.se/isk/research/doc/Birgitta_forum.pdf. An extensive summary from a Birgitta Forum held in August 2002 in Vadstena, Sweden, reviewed more in Emergent Genetic Information. Holzer, Jacqueline. Genomes & Language. http://www.liu.se/isk/research/doc/Birgitta_forum.pdf. A website for the conference program and lengthy Concluding Reflections from a Birgitta Forum held in August 2002 in Vadstena, Sweden. Geneticists and linguists are finding much commonality between these archetypal formative modes upon which our life and world is founded. A main resource is the work of the German philosopher Wolfgang Raible, who also spoke, Google for his 2001 paper “Linguistic and Genetics. Systematic Parallels”. Geneticists, when presenting the structure of the human genome, seem to find the metaphor of the genome as a book, or a text, useful. Genomes and texts are both multiply articulated structures, where purely contrastive units – phonemes, letters, bases – combine to form meaningful units at several levels of increasing complexity – words, sentences, texts; codons, genes, chromosomes. (4) In a very profound way he (Raible) shows the structural similarities between linguistics and genetics and sees herein a “deeper relationship between the ‘grammar of biology’ and the grammar of natural languages.” In both systems, the principles allowing the reconstruction of multi-dimensional wholes from linear sequences of basic elements are identical: double articulation, different classes of ‘signs,’ hierarchy, combinatorial rules: wholes are always more that the sum of their parts. (Holzer, 5)
Igamberdiev, Abir and Nikita Shklovskiy-Kordi.
Computational Power and Generative Capacity of Genetic Systems.
BioSystems.
142-143/1,
2016.
A Memorial University of Newfoundland theoretical biologist and a National Research Center for Hematology, Moscow research physician contribute to the intent of this journal (second quote) to achieve a natural philosophy of life’s evolution as an oriented ascent from an innately conducive cosmos. In this encompassing genesis, a “generative” agency is a textual essence which rises in kind from a physical matrix to genomic and linguistic manifestations. Once again, after decades of study, it is strongly put that these two prime codes are one and the same. Semiotic characteristics of genetic sequences are based on the general principles of linguistics formulated by Ferdinand de Saussure, such as the arbitrariness of sign and the linear nature of the signifier. Besides these semiotic features that are attributable to the basic structure of the genetic code, the principle of generativity of genetic language is important for understanding biological transformations. The problem of generativity in genetic systems arises to a possibility of different interpretations of genetic texts, and corresponds to what Alexander von Humboldt called “the infinite use of finite means”. These interpretations appear in the individual development as the spatiotemporal sequences of realizations of different textual meanings, as well as the emergence of hyper-textual statements about the text itself, which underlies the process of biological evolution. These interpretations are accomplished at the level of the readout of genetic texts by the structures, which includes DNA, RNA and the corresponding enzymes operating with molecular addresses. The molecular computer performs physically manifested mathematical operations and possesses both reading and writing capacities. Generativity paradoxically resides in the biological computational system as a possibility to incorporate meta-statements about the system, and thus establishes the internal capacity for its evolution. (Abstract)
Previous 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 Next
|
![]() |
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
HOME |
TABLE OF CONTENTS |
Introduction |
GENESIS VISION |
LEARNING PLANET |
ORGANIC UNIVERSE |