![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
|
![]() |
![]() |
||||||||||
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
|
VII. Our Earthuman Ascent: A Major Evolutionary Transition in Individuality1. Systems Physiology and Psychology: Somatic and Behavioral Development Friederici, Angela, et al. Maturation of the Language Network: From Inter- to Intrahemispheric Connectivities. PLoS One. 6/6, 2011. We note this work by Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences neuropsychologists because it describes youth to adult transitions from an earlier mode of cross-hemisphere communication to later more separate asymmetries. On measure, this sequence moves from an integral, right, balance to a left, detailed, verbose emphasis, which human cognitive history neatly seems to recapitulate. Language development must go hand-in-hand with brain maturation. Little is known about how the brain develops to serve language processing, in particular, the processing of complex syntax, a capacity unique to humans. Behavioral reports indicate that the ability to process complex syntax is not yet adult-like by the age of seven years. Here, we apply a novel method to demonstrate that the basic neural basis of language, as revealed by low frequency fluctuation stemming from functional MRI data, differs between six-year-old children and adults in crucial aspects. Although the classical language regions are actively in place by the age of six, the functional connectivity between these regions clearly is not. In contrast to adults who show strong connectivities between frontal and temporal language regions within the left hemisphere, children's default language network is characterized by a strong functional interhemispheric connectivity, mainly between the superior temporal regions. These data indicate a functional reorganization of the neural network underlying language development towards a system that allows a close interplay between frontal and temporal regions within the left hemisphere. (Abstract) Gao, Helena Hong and John Holland. Agent-Based Models of Levels of Consciousness. Santa Fe Institute Working Papers. 08-12-047, 2008. As part of a SFI project on language complexities (search Beckner), a Nanyang Technological University developmental linguist and the University of Michigan founder of complex adaptive systems theory perceive a six stage, infant to child, process by which reflective awareness arises: Unconscious (instinctive) activity, Minimal, Stimulus-response, Simple recursive (ask for food when visible), Extended recursive (I’m hungry), and Self-consciousness. A tacit assumption would seem to be our individual recapitulations of how primates evolved stepwise unto knowing utterances. This paper is based on recent interdisciplinary experimental studies that emphasize the steps in language acquisition during the first few years of life. These steps are characterized as changes in levels of consciousness. The object is to place successive levels of consciousness in a complex adaptive systems (CAS) framework, a framework that centers on learning agents that interact via exchanges of signals such as gestures and utterances. The CAS framework thus provides a strong emphasis on the social nature of language acquisition and evolution. The models described are exploratory, not predictive. As such, the models are meant to suggest new mechanisms and experiments that will increase our understanding of language. (Abstract) Gershkoff-Stowe, Lisa and Esther Thelen. U-Shaped Changes in Behavior: A Dynamic Systems Perspective. Journal of Cognition and Development. 5/1, 2004. As everyone is familiar with, we learn things, then forget or regress, and later on become more accomplished. These child psychologist authors find this ubiquitous path can be conceived as a self-organizing complex system which constantly seeks a better organization from a multitude of interacting modular-like influences. Gervain, Judit. The Role of Prenatal Experience in Language Development. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 21/62, 2018. This entry by a Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, CNRS, Paris linguist is able to report that our human proclivity for linguistic discourse is so strong it can be detected in fetal stages. An infant’s preference for holistic images can be traced back to receptions of rhythmic prosody even in the womb. Human infants are born linguistic citizens of the world, possessing broad-based, universal perceptual and learning abilities that allow them to start learning any language. After several months of experience, their linguistic system becomes tuned to the sound patterns of their native language(s). Recent results on newborns’ speech perception abilities suggest that this classical view might need to be nuanced, as fetuses seem to learn more from their prenatal experience with speech than previously believed. This paper reviews the growing body of evidence suggesting that newborns are familiar with the prosody of the languages heard in utero, and discusses the implications of this ‘prenatal prosodic bootstrapping’ for subsequent language acquisition. (Abstract) Ghalati, Pejman, et al. Critical Transitions in Intensive Care Units: A Sepsis Case Study. Nature Scientific Reports. 9/12888, 2019. We cite this contribution by a six person University of Aachen, Joint Research Center for Computational Biomedicine group as another example of how nonlinear physical phenomena can be found in forceful effect even in such physiological, and metabolic traumas. A novel ability to quantify these deep lineaments can then help predict and mitigate. The progression of complex human diseases is associated with critical transitions across dynamical regimes, which often provide early-warning signals and insights into disease-driving mechanisms. In this paper, we propose a computational method based on surprise loss (SL) to discover data-driven indicators of such transitions in a multivariate time series dataset of septic shock and non-sepsis patient cohorts. The core idea of SL is to train a mathematical model on time series in an unsupervised fashion and to quantify the deterioration of the model’s forecast (out-of-sample) relative to its past (in-sample) performance. Our analysis revealed that critical transitions occurred at a median of over 35 hours before the onset of septic shock, which validates our method as an early-warning indicator. (Abstract excerpt) Gottlieb, Gilbert, et al. The Significance of Biology for Human development: A Developmental Psychological Systems View. Lerner, Richard, vol. ed. Handbook of Child Psychology. 6th Edition. Vol. 1: Theoretical Models of Human Development. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, 2006. An historic revision is underway from preformation to epigenesis, from determinism to “probabilistic” organism-environment interaction from fetus to adolescent that serve to self-organize and create a unique person. This life process is said to be, in essence, an equifinality reachable by a variety of pathways. Grosberg, Anna, et al. Self-Organization of Muscle Cell Structure and Function. PloS Computational Biology. February 24, 2011. Harvard University, Wyss Institute, bioengineers provide sophisticated studies of one more instance of ubiquitous self-organized activities serving the growth and vitality of organismic life. Muscle morphogenesis is a hierarchal, self-organizing process spanning from nanometer scale conformational changes in proteins to bundled fibers sometimes a meter in length. We reasoned that boundary constraints are a physical signal that is conserved over all of these length scales and spatially organizes this broad range of coupled structures. Guidolin, Diego, et al. The “Self-Similarity Logic” Applied to the Development of the Vascular System. Developmental Biology. 351/156, 2011. University of Padova, Udine, and Bari medical morphologists here apply this mathematical insight originally posted in 2009 by neuroscientists Luigi Agnati, et al (search) to physiological purposes. We add in 2019 that the presence of such an innately universal repetition in kind has been found and robustly proven from human to universe. From a structural standpoint, living systems exhibit a hierarchical pattern of organization that is nested within one another. Recently, it has been suggested that some auto similarity prevails at each level or developmental stage and a principle of “self-similarity logic” has been proposed to convey the concept of a multi-level organization in which similar rules (logic) serve at each level. This study suggests that such a principle is likewise apparent in many morphological and developmental aspects of the vascular system. In fact, not only the morphology of the vascular system exhibits a high degree of geometrical self-similarity, but its remodelling processes also seem to be characterized by almost the same rules. (Abstract excerpt)
Hollenstein, Tom.
Twenty Years of Dynamic Systems Approaches to Development: Significant Contributions, Challenges, and Future Directions.
Child Development Perspectives.
5/4,
2011.
An Introduction to a special section on the subject whose articles by John Spencer, Alan Fogel, Paul van Geert, Marc Lewis, David Witherington, and other coauthors, with reference to Esther Thelen and Linda Smith, make this collection a valuable update survey of this nonlinear conceptual revolution for the field, in step with every other domain of universe and human. We offer quotes from select papers. Recent decades have seen a shift in thinking about development. Instead of characterizing what changes over development, there is a new emphasis on the how of developmental change. The explorations have revealed that simple notions of cause and effect are inadequate to explain development. Rather, change occurs within complex systems with many components that interact over multiple time scales, from the second-to-second unfolding of behavior to the longer time scales of learning, development, and evolution. (Spencer, et al, 260) A central challenge on the horizon for dynamic systems theory is to formally integrate across reciprocally interacting levels from genetic to social and to integrate these levels across multiple time scales from in-the-moment interactions to learning to development. (Spencer, et al, 263) Howe, Mark and Marc Lewis, eds. Development as Self-organization. Developmental Review. 25/3-4, 2005. A double issue on the importance of dynamic system approaches to fully understand somatic, cerebral and cognitive/social childhood maturation. Hu, Kun, et al. Fractal Patterns of Neural Activity Exist within the Suprachiasmatic Nucleus and Require Extrinsic Network Interactions. PLoS One. 7/11, 2012. We include this paper here by a Harvard Medical School, Leiden University Medical Centre, Netherlands, and Oregon Health & Science University team who have also been engaged in a similar studies of dynamic physiologies. See Pittman-Polletta, Benjamin, et al, below, who is also coauthor, for more reports. The mammalian central circadian pacemaker (suprachiasmatic nucleus, SCN) contains thousands of neurons that are coupled through a complex network of interactions. In addition to the established role of the SCN in generating rhythms of 24 hours in many physiological functions, the SCN was recently shown to be necessary for normal self-similar/fractal organization of motor activity and heart rate over a wide range of time scales—from minutes to 24 hours. To test whether the neural network within the SCN is sufficient to generate such fractal patterns, we studied multi-unit neural activity of in vivo and in vitro SCNs in rodents. In vivo SCN-neural activity exhibited fractal patterns that are virtually identical in mice and rats and are similar to those in motor activity at time scales from minutes up to 10 hours. Thus, SCN-neural activity is fractal in the intact organism and these fractal patterns require network interactions between the SCN and extra-SCN nodes. Such a fractal control network could underlie the fractal regulation observed in many physiological functions that involve the SCN, including motor control and heart rate regulation. (Abstract) Hunt, Nathaniel, et al. The Influence of Auditory-Motor Coupling on Fractal Dynamics in Human Gait. Nature Scientific Reports. 4/5879, 2014. University of Nebraska, and University College Dublin, biophysicists add novel perceptions that our bodily physiology and daily activity can indeed be described by and functions according to archetypal complex dynamics. As others have noted (search Havlin) these discoveries provide a guide to a person’s health which can be equated with how well one is attuned to critical rhythms between order and chaos. Fractal patterns observed in biological signals such as heart rate, respiration and walking strides measured over time, indicate that the time intervals between events are not equal, nor are they independent. Rather, there is a relationship between these intervals that extends far forward and backward in time; in other words, exhibiting long-range correlations in the time series, or fractal fluctuations. The presence of these fractal processes in biological systems is theoretically referred to as ‘‘complexity’’, which describes an underlying order or pattern that is contained within a complex, variable system; a system that is capable of sudden and marked change. The multi-scale fractal structure of the relationships between gait events is therefore thought to be ordered and stable, yet variable and flexible. Complexity is recognized as an inherent attribute of healthy biological systems, whereas the loss of complexity with aging and disease is thought to reduce the adaptive capabilities of the individual. A loss of complexity can refer to either an overly constrained, periodic system, or an overly random, incoherent system (1-2)
Previous 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 Next
|
![]() |
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
HOME |
TABLE OF CONTENTS |
Introduction |
GENESIS VISION |
LEARNING PLANET |
ORGANIC UNIVERSE |