|
IV. Ecosmomics: Independent, UniVersal, Complex Network Systems and a Genetic Code-Script Source2. The Innate Affinity of Genomes, Proteomes and Language Gimona, Mario. Protein Linguistics and the Modular Code of the Cytoskeleton. Barbieri, Marcello, ed. The Codes of Life. Berlin: Springer, 2008. The University of Salzburg geneticist contributes to the long project to interpret, join and unify the molecular and literal versions, in support of the growing conclusion that “Nature is Structured in a Language-like Fashion.” See also an earlier paper “Protein Linguistics – A Grammar for Modular Protein Assembly?” in Nature Reviews: Molecular Cell Biology (7/1, 2006). Hackenberg, Michael, et al. Clustering of DNA Words and Biological Function: A Proof of Principle. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 297/127, 2012. University of Granada and University of Malaga, Spain system biologists including Pedro Carpena contribute to historic 2010s verifications that the molecular nucleotide version and human cultural literature are one and the same, that they are formed and suffused by the same informative nonlinear complex network systems. View articles of this kind, for example, in the journal Complexity over recent years. Relevant words in literary texts (key words) are known to be clustered, while common words are randomly distributed. Given the clustered distribution of many functional genome elements, we hypothesize that the biological text per excellence, the DNA sequence, might behave in the same way: k-length words (k-mers) with a clear function may be spatially clustered along the one-dimensional chromosome sequence, while less-important, non-functional words may be randomly distributed. To explore this linguistic analogy, we calculate a clustering coefficient for each k-mer (k=2–9 bp) in human and mouse chromosome sequences, then checking if clustered words are enriched in the functional part of the genome. The clustering of DNA words thus appears as a novel principle to detect functionality in genome sequences. As evolutionary conservation is not a prerequisite, the proof of principle described here may open new ways to detect species-specific functional DNA sequences and the improvement of gene and promoter predictions, thus contributing to the quest for function in the genome. (Abstract excerpt) Heckmeier, Philipp, et al.. A billion years of evolution manifest in nanosecond protein dynamics. PNAS. 121/10, 2024. We cite this paper by University of Zurich and Columbia University biochemists as an example of how far the scope and range of these current techniques can reach. And again who are we peoples with an Earthomo sapience to be able to look down and back and reconstruct and re-present how it all came to occur? Protein dynamics forms a broad bridge between structure and function, yet the impact of evolution on ultrafast protein processes remains enigmatic. This study delves into the nanosecond-scale phenomena of a conserved protein across species separated by almost a billion years as a way to investigate ten complex homologs. In so doing, we found a cascade of rearrangements which manifest in discrete time points over hundreds of millions of years. Our work poses a novel scientific inquiry within molecular paleontology compared by the rapid pace of protein processes which can connect the shortest time scale in living matter (10^-9 s) with the largest ones (10^16 s). (Abstract) Holzer, Jacqueline. Genomes & Language. http://www.liu.se/isk/research/doc/Birgitta_forum.pdf. An extensive summary from a Birgitta Forum held in August 2002 in Vadstena, Sweden, reviewed more in Emergent Genetic Information. Holzer, Jacqueline. Genomes & Language. http://www.liu.se/isk/research/doc/Birgitta_forum.pdf. A website for the conference program and lengthy Concluding Reflections from a Birgitta Forum held in August 2002 in Vadstena, Sweden. Geneticists and linguists are finding much commonality between these archetypal formative modes upon which our life and world is founded. A main resource is the work of the German philosopher Wolfgang Raible, who also spoke, Google for his 2001 paper “Linguistic and Genetics. Systematic Parallels”. Geneticists, when presenting the structure of the human genome, seem to find the metaphor of the genome as a book, or a text, useful. Genomes and texts are both multiply articulated structures, where purely contrastive units – phonemes, letters, bases – combine to form meaningful units at several levels of increasing complexity – words, sentences, texts; codons, genes, chromosomes. (4) In a very profound way he (Raible) shows the structural similarities between linguistics and genetics and sees herein a “deeper relationship between the ‘grammar of biology’ and the grammar of natural languages.” In both systems, the principles allowing the reconstruction of multi-dimensional wholes from linear sequences of basic elements are identical: double articulation, different classes of ‘signs,’ hierarchy, combinatorial rules: wholes are always more that the sum of their parts. (Holzer, 5) Hwang, Yunha, et al. Genomic language model predicts protein co-regulation and function. Nature Communications.. 15/2880, 2024. We enter this work by Cornell, Harvard, Johns Hopkins, and MIT biologists including Sergey Ovchinnikov as another literate version of the textual affinity of nucleotides and narratives. See also ProteinEngine: Empower LLM with Domain Knowledge for Protein Engineering at arXiv:2405.06658. •
Igamberdiev, Abir and Nikita Shklovskiy-Kordi.
Computational Power and Generative Capacity of Genetic Systems.
BioSystems.
142-143/1,
2016.
A Memorial University of Newfoundland theoretical biologist and a National Research Center for Hematology, Moscow research physician contribute to the intent of this journal (second quote) to achieve a natural philosophy of life’s evolution as an oriented ascent from an innately conducive cosmos. In this encompassing genesis, a “generative” agency is a textual essence which rises in kind from a physical matrix to genomic and linguistic manifestations. Once again, after decades of study, it is strongly put that these two prime codes are one and the same. Semiotic characteristics of genetic sequences are based on the general principles of linguistics formulated by Ferdinand de Saussure, such as the arbitrariness of sign and the linear nature of the signifier. Besides these semiotic features that are attributable to the basic structure of the genetic code, the principle of generativity of genetic language is important for understanding biological transformations. The problem of generativity in genetic systems arises to a possibility of different interpretations of genetic texts, and corresponds to what Alexander von Humboldt called “the infinite use of finite means”. These interpretations appear in the individual development as the spatiotemporal sequences of realizations of different textual meanings, as well as the emergence of hyper-textual statements about the text itself, which underlies the process of biological evolution. These interpretations are accomplished at the level of the readout of genetic texts by the structures, which includes DNA, RNA and the corresponding enzymes operating with molecular addresses. The molecular computer performs physically manifested mathematical operations and possesses both reading and writing capacities. Generativity paradoxically resides in the biological computational system as a possibility to incorporate meta-statements about the system, and thus establishes the internal capacity for its evolution. (Abstract) Jolma, Arttu, et al. DNA-dependent Formation of Transcription Factor Pairs Alters Their Binding Specificity. Nature. 527/384, 2015. A Karolinska Institute, Sweden group and colleagues, led by Jussi Talpale, report a unique parsing of nucleotide genetics by treating them much as a linguistic script. The achievement was noted in a Science Daily item for November 15, 2015 (Google SD and article keywords) entitled Complex Grammar of the Genomic Language. A gene regulatory code is thus composed by “DNA words,” which can be seen to combine and compound just as lexicons and sentences. Karollus, Alexander, et al. Species-aware DNA language models capture regulatory elements and their evolution. Genome Biology.. Vol. 25/Art 83, 2024. In this BMC journal, Technical University of Munich geneticists introduce an effective synthesis of these premier nucleotide and narrative code-script domains. By so doing, a cross-assimilation is achieved of these biomolecular and linguistic text phases to an extent they can be seen as the same descriptive process in different sequential venues. See also How do Large Language Models understand Genes and Cells Chen Fang, et al in bioRxiv preprints for March 27, 2024 and Gene and RNA Editing at arXiv:2409.09057. Large-scale multi-species genome sequencing promises to shed new light on gene regulatory instructions. To this end, algorithms are needed that can leverage conservation while accounting for their evolution. Here, we introduce species-aware DNA language models trained on 800 species spanning 500 million years of evolution. We show that DNA language models distinguish transcription factor and RNA-binding protein motifs from background non-coding sequence. These results show that species-aware DNA language models are a powerful, flexible, and scalable tool to integrate information from large compendia of highly diverged genomes. (Abstract) Kay, Lily. A Book of Life?: How the Genome Became an Information System and DNA a Language. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 41/4, 1998. The late philosopher of science discerns intrinsic congruities between the verbal and genetic codes. Kay, Lily. Who Wrote the Book of Life? Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 2000. A premier history of science study of how a linguistic metaphor came to represent the genetic code. The author goes on to note a correspondence between molecular genetics, language and the Chinese divination system, the I Ching. As with (linguist Roman) Jakobson, the answer was affirmative (to the question of one basic code) and pointed to a universe fundamentally different from that portrayed in Jacques Monod’s Chance and Necessity. Rather than viewing DNA-based life as a product of chance, it would be chance subject to the structures and patterns of the I Ching. And rather than being a gypsy living on the edge of an alien world, as Monod decried, a human being would enjoy a deep sense of security that emerged from being planted physically and spiritually in an internal natural order. (318) Lackova, Ludmila, et al. Arbitrariness is not Enough: Towards a Functional Approach to the Genetic Code. Theory in Biosciences. Online May, 2017. Palacky University, Olomouc, Czech Republic linguists Lackova, Vladimir Matlach, and Dan Faltynek build a case for a semiotic definition of genomic conveyance. By this view, similar to written and oral communications, nucleotides and proteins are all about signs, symbols, interpretation and transcription. Apropos, from our home page a slide presentation, Cosmic Genesis in the 21st Century, that I gave at Palacky University in 2005 can be accessed. Arbitrariness in the genetic code is one of the main reasons for a linguistic approach to molecular biology: the genetic code is usually understood as an arbitrary relation between amino acids and nucleobases. However, from a semiotic point of view, arbitrariness should not be the only condition for definition of a code, consequently it is not completely correct to talk about “code” in this case. Semiotically, a code should be always associated with a function and we propose to define the genetic code not only relationally (in basis of relation between nucleobases and amino acids) but also in terms of function (function of a protein as meaning of the code). In fact, if the function of a protein represents the meaning of the genetic code (the sign’s object), then it is crucial to reconsider the notion of its expression (the sign) as well. In our contribution, we will show that the actual model of the genetic code is not the only possible and we will propose a more appropriate model from a semiotic point of view. (Abstract)
Previous 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 Next
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
HOME |
TABLE OF CONTENTS |
Introduction |
GENESIS VISION |
LEARNING PLANET |
ORGANIC UNIVERSE |