![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
|
![]() |
![]() |
||||||||||
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
|
VII. Our Earthuman Ascent: A Major Evolutionary Transition in Twindividuality3. A Complementary Brain and Thought Process: A Family Mind Bourne, Victoria. Lateralised Processing of Positive Facial Emotion: Sex Differences in Strength of Hemispheric Dominance. Neuropsychologia. 43/6, 2005. With regard to this task, men tend to use the right hemisphere while women draw on both sides of the brain. These findings are taken to imply that males are field-independent, females more field (context) dependent. The results suggest that females are more bilaterally distributed and hence have greater access to mechanisms located in each hemisphere. (953) Brosnan, Mark, et al. Gestalt Processing in Autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 45/3, 2004. Autistic children are said to lack a capacity for ‘gestalt grouping,’ distinguished here from overall global images. Dots are perceived but connections elude. Inter-element relationships, an ability to interact socially, is a consequent deficit. And while reading this article, one could not help think that our national mindset seems autistic as we descent into cognitive, rhetorical, and military chaos. As often cited after 9/11, we are unable to connect the dots and pieces into any meaningful pattern or salutary vision. There is a large body of literature that suggests the right hemisphere primarily subserves the processing underpinning gestalt and global analysis, while the left primarily subserves local analysis. (466) Buabang, Eike, et al. Leveraging cognitive neuroscience for making and breaking habits. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. November, 2024. Trinity College Dublin neuro-researchers including Claire Gillan achieve still another bicameral, bifocal rendition by the application of a dual process version (fast, fixed or slow, modified, search section) whereby a person’s active behavior is seen to mainly adopt these opposite modes, as the Abstract notes. Once again, the one, same me dot, We link and integral US viability becomes evident. As this late month is now taken over by more intense local and global polarities, it is so imperative that such scientific, cultural findings in our Earthuman midst comein to public light and benefit. Habits are behavioral complement of dual brain systems whereby a stimulus–response (S–R) mode repeats familiar activities while a goal-directed aspect is concerned with flexibility, prospection, and planning. Getting a proper balance between these two phases is crucial. Here we examine how recent understandings of these competing mechanisms can better manage both making and breaking habits. We discuss applications in everyday life, as well as validated interventions for clinical populations. In regard, we anticipate new opportunities for personalization of these interventions based on the neurobiology, environmental context, and personal preferences. (Excerpt) Chai, Lucy, et al. Functional Dynamics of the Language System. Cerebral Cortex. 26/11, 2016. In this section and elsewhere, are entered a growing number of citations about dual, reciprocal brain faculties such as fast and slow (Evans), creativity (Otis, Wolf), autism (Crespi), metastability (Kelso), and more. Here University of Pennsylvania and MIT neuroscientists including Evelina Fedorenko and Danielle Bassett report an iconic instance with regard to the processing of linguistic activities. While a “core” recognition of discrete words occurs in the left hemisphere, the right hemisphere provides the vital “peripheral” mode of content comprehension and meaningful value. During linguistic processing, a set of brain regions on the lateral surfaces of the left frontal, temporal, and parietal cortices exhibit robust responses. These areas display highly correlated activity while a subject rests or performs a naturalistic language comprehension task, suggesting that they form an integrated functional system. Evidence suggests that this system is spatially and functionally distinct from other systems that support high-level cognition in humans. Yet, how different regions within this system might be recruited dynamically during task performance is not well understood. Here we use network methods, applied to fMRI data collected from 22 human subjects performing a language comprehension task, to reveal the dynamic nature of the language system. We observe the presence of a stable core of brain regions, predominantly located in the left hemisphere, that consistently coactivate with one another. We also observe the presence of a more flexible periphery of brain regions, predominantly located in the right hemisphere, that coactivate with different regions at different times. By highlighting the temporal dimension of language processing, these results suggest a trade-off between a region's specialization and its capacity for flexible network reconfiguration. (Abstract)
Chaiken, Shelly and Yaacov Trope, eds.
Dual-Process Theory in Social Psychology.
New York: Guilford Press,
1999.
A large collection which addresses the reciprocal and sequential ways our brains learn, think and respond. Much theoretical writing but throughout these studies of attitudes, perceptions, social cognition and so on, a constant, interactive duality is reported of associative, emotional or rule-based, cognitive modes. As an overall observation, it is indicative of how specific disciplines such as Social Psychology are so isolated that nowhere in the book or index is there any mention of corresponding neural substrates, even though these dual properties align well with the right and left brain hemispheres. Chiron, C., et al. The Right Brain Hemisphere Is Dominant in Human Infants. Brain. 120/1057, 1997. The right hemisphere traits of visuospatial and emotional abilities develop earlier for purposes of individual and species survival. A child’s left brain begins its growth spurt at about two years of age. Christman, Stephen, et al. Individual Differences in Risk Perception versus Risk Taking. Brain and Cognition. 63/1, 2007. Right brain hemisphere processes are more sensitive and averse to risk than the left side. Women are thus more likely to avoid perilous situations than men. Interhemispheric interaction, more prevalent in women, is found to enhance ones ability in such regard. We make note of this work as another way to grasp the rash male dominance of our societies and cultures as they rush to violence and war with no thought to reason or consequence. Cook, Norman. Bihemispheric Language: How the Two Hemispheres Collaborate in the Processing of Language. Proceedings of the British Academy. 106/169, 2002. As understanding of the special functions of the left and right brain hemispheres grows in sophistication, their complementary interaction via the corpus callosum can be properly studied. The LH in general deals with auditory segmentation, denotation, close associations, literality, propositional meaning, explicit events and sequentialisation, while the RH handles intonational decoding, connotation, distance associations, metaphor, affective implications, implicit meaning (gist) and contextualization. What is observed is a left side penchant for more specific detail with the right taking a holistic survey, while in constant interplay. Cook, Norman. Tone of Voice and Mind: The Connections between Intonation, Emotion, Cognition and Consciousness. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing, 2003. A professor of informatics at Kansai University, Osaka, Japan, discusses the interplay of brain hemisphere propensities with regard to these qualities. Crespi, Bernard. Oxytocin, Testosterone, and Human Social Cognition. Biological Reviews. Online January, 2015. The Simon Fraser University evolutionary biologist presents a novel biochemical, hormonal basis for these complementary archetypal modes. As the Abstract explains, oxytocin fosters integrative personal relations which mediate and serve group cohesion, it is a feminine “bonding” hormone. Testosterone, the male anti-thesis, is a cause of much oppositional, disruptive behavior. Once again, from another source, these ultimately gender polarities can be identified. Of course, the resolve is always a balanced marriage of yin anima and yang animus See also Life in Groups: The Roles of Oxytocin in Mammalian Sociality by Allison Anacker and Annaliese Beery in Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience (7/186, 2013). How then might such palliative knowledge be identified and attributed to an emergent bicameral humankind so as to heal, free, and enhance the fraught creatures it arose from? I describe an integrative social-evolutionary model for the adaptive significance of the human oxytocinergic system. The model is based on a role for this hormone in the generation and maintenance of social familiarity and affiliation across five homologous, functionally similar, and sequentially co-opted contexts: mothers with offspring, female and male mates, kin groups, individuals with reciprocity partners, and individuals within cooperating and competing social groups defined by culture. In each situation, oxytocin motivates, mediates and rewards the cognitive and behavioural processes that underlie the formation and dynamics of a more or less stable social group, and promotes a relationship between two or more individuals. Such relationships may be positive (eliciting neurological reward, reducing anxiety and thus indicating fitness-enhancing effects), or negative (increasing anxiety and distress, and thus motivating attempts to alleviate a problematic, fitness-reducing social situation). I also present evidence that testosterone exhibits opposite effects from oxytocin on diverse aspects of cognition and behaviour, most generally by favouring self-oriented, asocial and antisocial behaviours. I apply this model for effects of oxytocin and testosterone to understanding human psychological disorders centrally involving social behaviour. (Abstract) Crespi, Bernard and Christopher Badcock. Psychosis and Autism as Diametrical Disorders of the Social Brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 31/241, 2008. Crespi is an evolutionary biologist at Simon Fraser University, while Badcock is a London School of Economics sociologist. The authors converge their respective research into a well-received synthesis which contends that these mental spectrums represent and ought to be best appreciated as polar extremes. If one may condense a rich article, they fall into archetypal categories of an attention to, or obsession with, either autistic particulate, insular detail or psychotic, manic/depressive associations. Respective genetic paternal and maternal influences can then be noted, while these typical behaviors go on to define an extreme masculine or feminine dichotomy. By virtue of such insights, along with many other contributions herein, the universal gender complements reappear, which again evoke an ever elusive middle balance. The paper merits a long quote from its abstract. Autistic-spectrum conditions and psychotic-spectrum conditions (mainly schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depression) represent two major suites of disorders of human cognition, affect, and behavior that involve altered development and function of the social brain. We describe evidence that a large set of phenotypic traits exhibit diametrically opposite phenotypes in autistic-spectrum versus psychotic-spectrum conditions, with a focus on schizophrenia. This suite of traits is inter-correlated, in that autism involves a general pattern of constrained overgrowth, whereas schizophrenia involves undergrowth. These disorders also exhibit diametric patterns for traits related to social brain development, including aspects of gaze, agency, social cognition, local versus global processing, language, and behavior. Social cognition is thus underdeveloped in autistic-spectrum conditions and hyper-developed on the psychotic spectrum.
Crespi, Bernard, et al.
Imagination in Human Social Cognition, Autism, and Psychotic-Affective Conditions.
Cognition.
150/181,
2016.
Simon Fraser University and University of Alberta bioneuroscientists advance their insights into a psychological continuum from autistic behaviors unable to make connections to disorders of over-perceived imagery. As much research attests, autism is a male condition which fixates on separate pieces or dots without any object, or personal interrelations. At the other end, schizophrenics are known to obsess over realities not in actual existence. These states appear to exhibit directly-opposite patterns with regard to convergent versus divergent thinking, and restricted interests and insistence on sameness versus novelty, fantasy, goal-seeking in mania, and openness. (184) Complex human social cognition has evolved in concert with risks for psychiatric disorders. Recently, autism and psychotic-affective conditions (schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, depression) have been posited as psychological ‘opposites’ with regard to social-cognitive phenotypes. Imagination, considered as ‘forming new ideas, mental images, or concepts’, represents a central facet of human social evolution and cognition. Previous studies have documented reduced imagination in autism, and increased imagination in association with psychotic-affective conditions, yet these sets of findings have yet to be considered together, or evaluated in the context of the diametric model. We first review studies of the components, manifestations, and neural correlates of imagination in autism and psychotic-affective conditions.
Previous 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 Next [More Pages]
|
![]() |
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
HOME |
TABLE OF CONTENTS |
Introduction |
GENESIS VISION |
LEARNING PLANET |
ORGANIC UNIVERSE |